• @roastpotatothief@lemmy.ml
        link
        fedilink
        1
        edit-2
        3 years ago

        it’s not so easy to explain. look up Wikipedia, or even better look up Sally Clark. she’s the textbook example, and an amazing story anyway.

        • @tomtom@lemmy.ml
          link
          fedilink
          13 years ago

          pretty interesting, thx.

          here is an edited selection from wikipedia for posterity:

          Mathematically, the fallacy results from misunderstanding the concept of a conditional probability, which is defined as the probability that an event A occurs given that event B is known – or assumed – to have occurred, and it is written as P(A|B).

          The error is based on assuming that P(A|B) = P(B|A).

          For example, let A represent the event of getting a haircut, and B the event of reading a haircut meme.

          But this equality is not true: in fact, although P(A|B) is usually very small, P(B|A) may still be much higher.

          • @roastpotatothief@lemmy.ml
            link
            fedilink
            1
            edit-2
            3 years ago

            No I don’t think that’s it. I would express it this way.

            The chance of me reading a haircut meme straight after getting a haircut is near zero. But there are multiple people reading the haircut meme. So if you post a haircut meme, there is a high chance that ANY ONE of the readers will be straight after getting a haircut.


            Like Sally Clark was convicted because the chance of having two miscarriages in a row is a million to one - it’s not plausible. The only other explanation is that she murdered her babies, so that’s the only plausable explanation.

            But in a population of 60 million people, you are likely to find someone (or 60 people) who have had two miscarriages in a row. She was just the unlucky one.

            Wikipedia has more detail and intersting angles, like the defendent’s fallacy, why it is a fallacy and when it might be correct.


            Then there’s more stuff, and what happened after the conviction… That pathologist was never even charged with a crime, or lynched.


            edit:

            But your point is interesting too. The chance of winning the lottery given that I’ve just had a coffee is very low. But the chance that I’ve just had a coffee given that I’ve won the lottery is very high. But if I drink a coffee and then immediately win the lottery, people might assume that having just had a coffee improves my lottery chances.

            I don’t know if there is a name for that one, but it sounds like something people do a lot.

            I get sick, take medicine, get better. Then chance of having taken medicine given that I get better is 100%. So people will assume the medicine helps. But the chance that I get better given that I’ve taken medicine might be 10%. The chance I get better if I had not taken medicine might be more than 10%, but that result is not obvious at all.

            • @tomtom@lemmy.ml
              link
              fedilink
              13 years ago

              Ah your explanation clears it up. That whole conditional probability thing is in the wikipedia article, but I see now that my explanation of the haircut thing was not correct.

              I guess maybe this is a better formulation:

              p1 = P(not being guilty | evidence found)

              vs

              p2 = P(evidence found)

              Prosecutor’s fallacy would assert that, if p2 is small say 0.01%, then the defendent is guilty. But really the relevant probability is p1, which could be quite a bit larger than 0.01%.

              Anyways let me know if you agree lol.